Архитектура Аудит Военная наука Иностранные языки Медицина Металлургия Метрология
Образование Политология Производство Психология Стандартизация Технологии


Abbreviations Used in Traffic Handling



 

 


AA all after                                               

AB all before you                                     

ADR address                                            

BN all between                                         

CFM confirm                                            

CK check                                                  

DLD delivered                                          

GBA give better address                          

MSG prefix to radiogram                         

NIL nothing; I have nothing for

PBL preamble

REF refer to

SINE opr's personal initials

TXT text

VC prefix to service message

WA word after

WB word before

WD word

 


 

Examples Of The Phillips Code
The Phillips code was developed to bring the sending operator's skill up toward that of the receiving operator, who typically could receive much faster than anyone could send by hand. Phillips code is a systematic, rigid system of abbreviations used along with normal spelling of all other words, and cuts total transmission time about in half. There were about 6000 abbreviations under this long-used system. It was used in commercial press (news) transmissions. A skilled operator could easily keep up, typing out the words in full as fast as the sender could hand send, but he didn't dare let his mind wander.

 

Words were cut to their "backbone", leaving only the letters that carry the brunt of their pronunciation. See the list below for how this was done. Abbreviated words were modified by, e.g., adding "d" for the past tense of verbs and "g" for -ing; "s" was added to nouns for their plural; some words added "b" forable. A couple of simple examples of text are given here.

 

Example of 188 letters reduced to 116 (?) 61.7%:
"T DCN CD MEAN T END F UNPRECEDENTED TWO Y CDY BTL, T FS D US X A SURROGATE MOTHER WS TKN TO TRL FO BACKING OUT O AN AGM TO TURN OV A CHILD SHE BORE UND CAK." Translated into normal text, it says:

 

"The decision could mean the end of the unprecedented two year custody battle, the first in the United States in which a surrogate mother was taken to trial for backing out of an agreement to turn over a child she bore under contract."

 


AB about

ABV above

ADZ advise

AF after

AG again

AJ adjust

ANR another

AR answer

AX ask

AY any

AYG anything

AYM any more

B be

BC because with

BD board

BF before

BH both

BK break

BN been

BTN between

BTR better

C see

CCN conclusion

CD could

CK check

CKT circuit

CL call

CLO close

CLR clear

CMB combine

CNG change

CT connect

CU current

CY copy

D in the

DD did

DT do not

DUX duplex

EMGY emergency

EQ equip

EQPT equipment

F of the

FD find

FJ found

FM from

FR for

FT for the

FYI for your information

G from the

GD good


 


In addition to these there were a large number of very short special abbreviations for phrases common in news releases, such as for "President of the United States," etc. Usually these consisted of 3 - 5 letters, very brief.

 

 


GG going

GM gentleman

GTG getting

GV give

H has

HD had

HM him

HR here, hear

HS his

HV have

HW how

ICW in connection

IM immediately

INVG investigate

IX it is

KW know

LV leave

M more

MK make

MSG missing

MSJ message

MSR measure

N not

NA name

NF notify

NI night

NR near

NTG nothing

NUM number

NV never

NW now

NX next

OD order

OFS office

OP operate

OTR other

OV over

PGH paragraph

PLS please

Q on the

QK quick

S send

SAF soon as feasible

SAP soon as possible

SD should

SED said

SES says

SM some

SM somehere

SMG something

SN soon

SNC since

SPL special

STN station

SVL several

T the

TGH telegraph

TGR together

TI time

TK take

TM them

TNK think

TRU through

TS this

TT that

TTT that the

TW tomorrow

TY they

U you

UN until

UR your

VY very

W with

WD would

WG wrong

WH which

WI will

WIN within

WIT witness

WK week

WN when

WO who

WR were

WS was

WT what

WY why

YA yesterday


 

A Few Useful Z- Signals
The Z-signals were developed and used for a time by some commercial operators. A few of them which might be useful to amateurs are:

 


ZCG local receiving conditions good
ZCP local receiving conditions poor
ZLS we are suffering from a lightning storm
ZSH static is heavy here

ZOK we are receiving OK
ZSR your sigs strong readable
ZGS your signals are getting stronger
ZWR your sigs weak but readable
ZFS your signals are fading slightly
ZVS signals varying in intensity
ZFB your signals are fading badly
ZGW your signals are getting weaker
ZSU your sigs are unreadable
ZAN we can receive absolutely nothing
ZUB we are unable to break you

ZVF your signals are varying in frequency
ZDH your dits are too heavy (long), please adjust
ZDL your dits are too light(short), please adjust
ZMO stand by a moment
ZMQ stand by for...
ZLB give long breaks
ZWO send words once
ZWT send words twice
ZSF send faster
ZSS send slower
ZTH send by hand
ZCS cease sending
ZAP acknowledge please

ZHC how are your receiving conditions?
ZRO are you receiving OK?


 

You will notice that these signals are much easier to remember than the Q-signals. The two letters following Z- are suggestive. (QST 1943 No p. 63)

 

In 1910 some wireless abbreviations were:
GA= go ahead, 4= please start me, where..., 13= understand,
25= am busy now, 30= no more, 77= message for you, 99= keep out!











































Chapter 28

Letter Frequency Counts

-International Morse-

 

The letter frequency counts (left-most column) are taken from one of the common books on cryptanalysis, based on number of occurrences per thousand of normal English text material. Each character is analyzed ("structure") into units, 1 for minimum signal duration (one dit), 111 (three units duration) for a dah, and each equal unit of silence denoted by 0 (zero). The required three units of silence separating each character is added (000) to each one below.

 

Freq.   Letter        Structure                     Units   Total
130       E                 1000                               4           520
92         T                 111000                           6           552
79         N                11101000                       8           632
76         R                1011101000                   10         760
75         O                11101110111000           14         1050
74         A                10111000                       8           592
74         I                  101000                           6           444
61         S                 10101000                       8          488
42         D                1110101000                   10         420
36         L                 101110101000               12         432
34         H                1010101000                   10         340
31         C                11101011101000           14         434
28         F                 101011101000               12         336
27         P                 10111011101000           14         378
26         U                1010111000                   10         260
25         M                1110111000                   10         250
19         Y                1110101110111000       16         304
16         G                111011101000               12         192
16         W               101110111000               12         192
15         V                101010111000               12         180
10         B                111010101000               12         120
5           X                11101010111000           14         70
3           Q                1110111010111000       16         48
3           K                111010111000               12         36
2           J                  1011101110111000       16         32
1           Z                 11101110101000           14         14

 

1000 Ave. Structure length 11.23 Ave. 9.07 9076

 

From the above, if we take five times the above average letter length and add the space required for word spacing (seven total or 0000000) we arrive at the normal English word length as 5 x 9.076 + 4 = 49.38. This is just a bit less than 1% shorter than 50 units per standard word. (By contrast, a random five-letter group averages 60.15 units. This is 20.3% longer than normal
English word length.) A similar analysis of numbers will show that the average length of a number is 17 units (minimum 12, maximum 22) or a group of five numbers takes about 1.78 times as long to transmit as a five letter word.

 

Comparing these calculations will show some of the reasons why receiving speeds vary with the kind of material being sent.

 

As a matter of interest, we list here the letters from the shortest to the longest by the number of units (less letter space) -- notice that all lengths are odd numbers: 1 - E; 3 - I, T; 5 - A, N, S; 7 - D, H, M, R, U; 9 - B, F, G, K, L, V, W; 11 - C, O, P, X, Z; 13 - J, Q, Y.

 

Foreign Adaptations Of The International Morse Code:
If the same kind of calculations are carried out for several foreign languages, the following results are obtained for the average character length: (Frequency data from Secret and Urgent, Fletcher Pratt l942 Tables II to IV, p. 253 ff.) German 8.640, French 8.694, Spanish 8.286 . These range on the average from 5 - 9% shorter per character than in English. There seems little doubt that if the code were somewhat redesigned and adjusted to optimize it for English a reduction of about 5% could be made.

 

For the Original American Morse code:


Mr. Ivan Coggeshall made an analysis of American Morse comparatively, using the same normal dah lengths and word spacings one unit shorter, and arrived at an average letter (frequency) length of 7.978 (as compared with 9.076) and average number length of l4. As noted in Chapter 16, American Morse timing is open to considerable variation.

 

 

 






























Chapter 29

The Koch Researches

 

 

The obviously extensive researches of Ludwig Koch, psychologist at Die technische Hochschule, Braunschweig, Germany, reported in Jan-Feb. 1936 (see Sources), seem to be virtually unknown outside of Germany. His goal was to discover the most efficient way to teach the Morse code to prospective radiotelegraph operators to meet the International requirements for commercial radio operators. These requirements were:

 

· send 100 words in five minutes,

· copy a 100 word telegram in five minutes, and

· copy 125 words of ordinary text in five minutes, one word being reckoned as five letters

 

Koch's researches involved: determining what competent operators are doing, examining teaching methods in current use, then devising better methods, and testing them in actual classes. His conclusions and recommendations seem to be the earliest real research into how best to teach the Morse code. They agree on the whole with the best methods of today, and may offer some further ideas of value to us. They are summarized here.

 

Tests to Determine What Competent Operators Are Doing
He ran three series of tests to determine how the code is comprehended and for this purpose used four competent, actively-practicing radio telegraphers. Three of these operators had learned the code solely by sound, while the fourth was self-taught from printed code charts.

 

Sending Tests
For the first test each operator was to send by regular handkey the series of ten letters b c v q f l h y z x at various speeds while monitoring his sending with a pair of headphones to satisfy himself as to its quality. Out of his sight and hearing a recording system made an accurate timed graphical record of his sending, so that the actual timing of signal and space durations could be examined in detail. He was instructed to send, using standard International Morse timing, at each of six different speeds ranging from about 20 to 80 characters per minute. Standard International Morse timing, as described in Chapter 12, was then used to compare their sending at all speeds.

 

Below about 10 wpm the only operator who closely conformed to standard timing was the one who had visually learned the code. The three others deviated considerably from "standard" timing. At 5 wpm these deviations were appreciable:

 

· the dits were too short,

· the dahs tended to be longer than 3 times dit length, and

· the spaces between characters were too long.

 

However, spacing between the components of a letter was almost perfectly equal to their dit lengths.

 

At successively higher speeds this situation changed slowly and somewhat irregularly until by about 10-wpm character rate all four operators were forming fairly accurate patterns of sound (nearly to the International Standard), except that the letters themselves were somewhat faster and the spaces between letters were somewhat longer than standard. By about 12-wpm all sending had become quite consistent with the standard. (Only the well-known individual peculiarities of sending by hand were obvious. At 10-wpm and above these deviations were always very small.)

 

The three operators who had learned by sound obviously showed no real sense of sound patterning (Gestalt) at these very low speeds: no sense of unity, but rather just a series of separate elements strung together. Only by about 10 wpm were the code characters now felt to be entities of sound in themselves, patterns which were clear-cut in each operator's mind, no longer shattered elements, disjointed parts.

 

Receiving Tests
Test number One: - Each operator was to copy the 30 German Morse characters sent by a machine in perfect "standard" timing at each of four different speeds over the same speed range as before.

 

At about 5-wpm these experienced operators hardly recognized a single character correctly! At 7-wpm only 40% to 60% of the letters were correctly identified. At 10-wpm all operators were getting about 95% correct. By 12 wpm all of them correctly identified every character.

Test number Two: - Here the length of the spaces between the letters was doubled. This time the operators recognized almost all letters correctly at all speeds. That is interesting.

From these tests it was concluded that experienced operators recognize a code character by its overall acoustic pattern (Gestalt), and that this pattern stands out clearly only when sent at a minimum character speed of about 50 characters per minute. At lower speeds it is heard simply as a disjointed series of signals. -- Koch concluded that these operators could recognize the too-slowly sent letters only when letter spacing was doubled, because this increased interval gave them time to integrate the sound and mentally speed it up to where they could recognize it. (A beginner would not have the skill to do this.)

The operator who had learned from a printed code chart apparently formed better-proportioned characters at very low speeds because his visual mental picture was so strong. However, the price paid for this was that it limited his maximum speed of copying: he could barely meet the minimum requirements - a marginal operator. (See below.)

 

Analysis And Criticism Of Previous Teaching Methods
The "Analytic" Method introduces the student to the code using some sort of systematic arrangement, or chart, where the code characters are arranged by number and type of related elements, etc., in a visual form. The student is required to memorize this as a mental picture before going any further. After that, the characters are sent to him in standard timing, at first very, very slowly. This means they are sent with long drawn out dits, dahs and spaces. The speed is then very gradually increased in tiny steps.

 

The faults with this system are:

· To begin by learning visual symbols creates a useless detour

· Slow sending destroys any unity, or coherent sound-patterning

· The disjointed signal doesn't meet our need for a sense of unity

· Learner can hardly help counting the dits and dahs

· the long spaces between letters distract his attention from listening by:

· encouraging him to think and try to put the shattered parts together to make sense of them, a shaped unity, (Gestalt), or

· guessing what may come next

· at each increase in speed everything sounds different, and he virtually has to start over again.

In short, the student is sidetracked and severely penalized all the way along: needlessly translating from bits and pieces of sound to try to put it together into a meaningful whole, then converting that to visual form and then finally to the letter.

The "Sound-Pattern" Method first introduces the Morse characters to the student at a character speed fast enough for them to be perceived as an acoustic unity (Gestalt), but with wide spaces between the characters. However, the student has usually already visually mastered a code table or is encouraged to do so as he learns.

 

Unfortunately, visual mental pictures are usually very much stronger and easier to recall than auditory sound patterns. Thus the student tends to convert the signal pattern he hears into the corresponding visual representation, break it into its component parts, and then finally into the letter. This complex action tends at least partially destroys the wholeness of the acoustic impression.

 

This series of actions is encouraged by the long pauses between characters, giving adequate time for thinking, speculation and the cumbersome translation processes. With increasing speeds the pause time becomes too short to go through all this, and so the student gets stuck below or around 10 wpm, just as with the analytic method.

 

So this method tends to suffer about the same faults as the analytical method. Both generally lead directly to that troublesome plateau at around 10 wpm, where the distinct change in perception from bits and pieces to coherent unity of each signal occurs.

 

Analyzing these methods, two classes of errors can be seen:

 

· Errors which hinder the building of a sense of acoustic unity

· Detour through an optical symbol.

· Disintegration of the acoustic form of the character.

· Errors which prevent going directly from acoustic impression to the letter:

· Thinking about the signal during long pauses.

· Guessing what may come next.

· Converting or translating from sound to visual and from visual to the letter

· Converting or integrating into a total rhythm pattern.

 

The remedy is obviously to eliminate all visual references and associate the sound directly with the letter, to send fast enough from the very beginning so that coherent sound patterns are immediately sensed, and to eliminate non-normal spacing between letters.

 


Поделиться:



Последнее изменение этой страницы: 2019-04-19; Просмотров: 187; Нарушение авторского права страницы


lektsia.com 2007 - 2024 год. Все материалы представленные на сайте исключительно с целью ознакомления читателями и не преследуют коммерческих целей или нарушение авторских прав! (0.062 с.)
Главная | Случайная страница | Обратная связь